Paradoxical Sleep - A Study of its Nature and Mechanisms
Michel Jouvet
Progress In Brain Research Vol. 18 Sleep Mechanisms 1965
TABLE OF CONTENTS
Introduction
Evidence of the duality of the states of sleep

(a) EEG and behavioural findings

(b) Phylogenetic findings

(c) Ontogenetic findings

(d) Functional findings

(e) Structural findings

Mechanisms of paradoxical sleep

(a) Producing P.S. as a reflex

(b) Results of deafferentations

(c) Role of the hypothalamus and pituitary

(d) Deprivation of P.S. in the pontile animal

(e) Effects of temperature on P.S. in the pontile animal

(f) Action of gamma-butyrolactone (G.B.L.)

(g) Osmolarity of the blood and paradoxical sleep

Discussion

(a) Duality of the states of sleep

(b) Mechanisms underlying the appearance of P.S.

Summary and Conclusions

Discussion

Figures

PRINT
Printable version

Discussion

(b) Mechanisms underlying the appearance of P.S.

We shall discuss here the question of reflex production of P.S., the effect of deprivation of P.S. in pontile animals and humoral factors.

(i) Reflex production of P.S.

Inhibition of nuchal tonus during P.S. is not dependent on the gamma system alone, since it persists after section of the posterior cervical roots of the intact animal. Since, furthermore, the cerebellum is intact in pontile animals, it would appear that production of P.S. as a reflex depends on the integrity of the pontine reticular formation (posterior part of the nucleus reticularis pontis oralis and anterior part of the N.R.P. caudalis). There is thus no direct inhibitory effect emanating from the bulbar reticular formation (situated at the level of the nucleus giganto-cellularis and nucleus reticularis ventralis (Magoun and Rhines, 1946; brodal, 1957), since P.S. can no longer be triggered by mechanisms distal to the mediopontine or retropontine areas. Participation of adrenalin secretion can also be ruled out, as P.S. can appear immediately after nociceptive stimulation and, furthermore, injection of adrenalin does not produce P.S. It also appears that the integrity of the mesencephalic tegmentum prevents reflex P.S. in mesencephalic animals, although P.S. still occurs spontaneously. Activation of the descending facilitatory system (Magoun, 1950) would thus preclude descending inhibition in mesencephalic animals. We did not analyse the nervous effects of the natural stimuli used in our experimentation, so that it is difficult to isolate the particular type of afferents responsible for triggering P.S. But we do know that the majority of afferents of group III project principally on to the mesencephalic tegmentum, while those of groups I and II project for the most part on to the pontine reticular formation (Pompeiano and Swett, 1963), so that the latter may be responsible for producing reflex P.S.

Thus, under certain conditions, P.S. can be produced by a reflex mechanism. A distinction must however be drawn between generalized atony, a veritable reflex cataplexy, without pontine spikes or 'spindles' and eye movements, and true P.S. The former state presents no refractory phase and can follow any proprioceptive, or nociceptive, stimulus when the pons has been transected at the nucleus reticularis pontis oralis level. Such cataplexy resembles the 'sudden postural collapse' described by Bard and Macht (1958). It has never been possible, on the other hand, to obtain an iterative form of P.S. Thus, the data invite the conclusion that there exist two different systems at the level of the pons. The first, which is not endowed with refractory period, is responsible for inhibition of muscular tonus (probably via the inhibitory bulbar reticular formation), and the second, which controls the first and presents a refractory period, is responsible for pontine spikes and 'spindles' (and probably the EEG phenomena in intact animals) and for the eye movements. This second system can be identified with the 'centre déclenchant' of P.S. (M. Jouvet, 1962a) for this is the level at which the refractory phase producing the periodicity of P.S. appears. Such a system may explain the different aspects of certain reflex cataplectic attacks in humans. The one type - cataplexy - narcolepsy - presents EEG, polygraphic and electromyographic tracings identical with those in s]eep with eye movements (personal observation), and the other cataplexy - is characterized by intact consciousness.

If, therefore, P.S. can be induced as a reflex under certain conditions, this does not appear to be the sole mechanism. Nevertheless, this hypothesis has been maintained by Lissak et al. (1962). These authors contend that inhibition of muscular tonus is the cause, and not the effect, of P.S. But several facts make it possible to preclude the exclusive activation of P.S. by peripheral muscular afferents.

(1) Cortical activation of P.S. often precedes the disappearance of muscular tonus in intact animals.

(2) In pontile animals (without hypothalamic island) long periods of total atony are not infrequently observed. No EMG activity in the neck or other muscles is then present. ]n spite of this complete atony, P.S. can regularly appear, recognizable by pontine EEG criteria, rapid eye movements and autonomic changes. Total muscular atony is thus not per se an inhibitory or facilitatory factor of P.S.

(3) Lastly, total section of the spinal cord at C6 with section of the brachial plexus, whereby a large number of afferents of muscular origin are suppressed, does not affect the appearance of P.S.

Thus, the result of nervous deafferentation experiments is negative. Furthermore, the periodic and regular appearance of P.S. 4 5 days after ablation of the hypothalamus and pituitary rules out a direct and indispensable participation of ACTH and the post-pituitary hormones and the different hypothalamic neurohormones. The decrease in P.S. seen from about the fourth day and before death must be ascribed to delayed ionic and metabolic disturbances resulting from the prolonged absence of pituitary (especially antidiuretic) hormones.

The periodic appearance of P.S. thus cannot be explained by peripheral nervous or hypothalamo-pituitary factors, so that investigation can be restricted to pontine phenomena (neuronal or glial), although peripheral humoral factors cannot be excluded a priori.

(ii) It is difficult with the methods of study employed to specify the mechanisms of P.S. precisely. But several indirect findings all point to this state of sleep being endowed with a certain 'autorhythmicity' and situated in the pons.

The presence of P.S. under conditions of hypothermia at 29° indicates the resistance to cold of its underlying mechanisms. The findings are similar to those in anesthesia: P.S. in fact continues to appear periodically in decorticated animals after doses of 20-30 mg/kg pentobarbitone (M. Jouvet, 1962a). Furthermore, the greater duration of P.S. (and of the intervals between the phases) during hypothermia is reminiscent of enzymatic processes and suggests a metabolic phenomenon.

The action of y-butyrolactone (G.B.L.) is difficult to interpret, in spite of the fact that its structure is simple. Bessman and Skolnik (1964) see G.B.L. as a normal constituent of the brain, an increase in the intracerebral concentration coinciding with anesthesia induced by G.B.L. or gamma-hydroxybutyrate (G.H.B.). Giarman and Roth (1964), on the other hand, found no G.B.L. in the blood or brain and attribute the anesthetic role to y-hydroxybutyrate. G.B.L. does not appear to be a precursor of cerebral GABA, for the level of the latter does not increase after injection of G.B.L ( Giarman and Schmidt, 1963). It has also been shown that injection of G.B.L. can raise the level of acetylcholine in the brain (Giarman and Schmidt, 1963), and specifically in the area of the corpora quadrigemina. Whilst the mechanism behind this increase in cerebral acetylcholine is not known the ata suggest a cholinergic-type link within the P.S. process. Certain peripheral manifestations of P.S. in intact animals (myosis, bradycardia, increased and irregular respiration, arterial hypotension (Candia et al., 1962) inhibitory role of atropine (M. Jouvet, 1962a), support this view. But while such observations suggest the intervention of cholinergic neurons during P.S., they cannot explain the periodicity.

(iii) Deprivation of P.S.

The progressive diminution of the intervals between each incipient phase of P.S. in pontile cats caused by the increasingly frequent repetition of the disturbing shock, suggests a biochemical process whereby the accumulation during wakefulness of an unknown factor among the metabolites of neuronal activity provokes the increasingly rapid induction of a recuperative process. The inescapable nature of this reappearance of P.S. meant that with our technique prolonged deprivation was impossible. It nevertheless appears that a minimal increase in the recuperative process is adequate to restore the earlier conditions and normal rhythm of P.S.

In the intact animal, on the other hand, in which prolonged periods of deprivation are possible, P.S. increases considerably but does not exceed a certain ceiling (60%) during the first 6 h of recuperative sleep. This plateau thus appears to represent the upper limit of the cyclic metabolic processes of P.S. In this connection, it is interesting to note that recuperation does not occur in one long phase of P.S., but the periodicity remains, as though it were impossible for P.S. to last for more than 20-25 min at a stretch. Thus, the P.S. deficit which is accumulated over a long period of deprivation can only be partially and slowly compensated by a phenomenon which remains periodical and is of limited duration. This suggests the existence of a self-regulating cyclic metabolic process which requires several days before it can 'neutralize' the unknown factor accumulated during deprivation.

(iv) It is a difficult task to interpret the relationships between osmolarity of the blood and P.S., for the amounts of ions normally present in the cerebrospinal fluid and the brain are not known. The post-pituitary hormone could be cited as an indirect factor in the increase of P.S. during withdrawal of liquids and following injection of hypertonic saline solution. But injection of ADH does not promote P.S., which can occur in transitory diabetes insipidus (i.e. in the absence of post-pituitary hormones). There must therefore be another mechanism at work, which cou]d be none other than a direct influence of the osmolarity of the blood. In the case of pontile animals with an intact hypothalamic island, the relationship between the blood and the brain must always be considered in particular at the level of the barrier between the capillaries and the neuroglial cells, to which an important and active role is accredited in what is termed the blood-brain barrier (Edström, 1964). According to recent findings (De Robertis and Gerschenfeld, 1961), the neuroglia appears to be the tissue that plays the greatest part in the mechanisms of ion and water exchange between the internal milieu and the neurons. It constitutes a water ion pool (Gerschenfeld et al., 1959) between the blood and the neurons, playing a part in the transportation of metabolites and the storage or elimination of K and Na at the neuronal level. An active barrier, which has been likened to the renal glomeruli (Tschirgt, 1958; Edström, 1964), would thus keep the whole neuronal complex relatively independent of variations in the iOI1 content of the extracellular fluid. Furthermore, the osmolarity of the brain is closely related to that of the blood and quickly adjusts itself to variations in the latter (Stern and Coxon, 1964). There are thus grounds for supposing that variations in the osmolarity of the blood above all affect the glial cells. lt is therefore possible that an electro]yte concentration exceeding that of these cells facilitates the enzymatic mechanisms responsible for P.S., whereas a lower concentration would inhibit them. Lastly, the appearance of regular rhythmic activity during rehydration (after fluid deprivation) only during P.S. would appear to signify a close relationship between the electrical activity of the pons in P.S. and the osmolarity of the blood. One might therefore presume that at this moment, and only at this moment, the glial structures participate in the recurrence of a certain type of cerebral homeostasis. This hypothetical periodic glial mechanism, with its purpose either of maintaining cerebral homeostasis (cerebrostasis) or as an active process for the elimination of certain metabolites, would protect the brain so that P.S. would appear as a periodic phenomenon interrupting waking activity in the pontile animal and slow sleep in the intact animal, to restore equilibrium at the neuronal level. It must be admitted that if such a mechanism exists its location in the pons would be ideal. For, at that level, the nervous and glial cells could receive biochemical information on the activity of the neurons from the whole nervous system, since the afferents from the rostral and caudal regions of the nervous system converge at that point (Brodal, 1957), whilst efferent ascending and descending pathways lead out from the pons to the cortex and spinal cow (Scheibel and Scheibel, 1957).

(v) The nature of P.S.

Finally, we must gather the facts together and attempt to draw up some hypotheses on the nature of P.S.

It would appear to be established that P.S. is dependent on a periodic mechanism of an unknown nature situated in the pons. This mechanism triggers an ascending tonic neuronal activity similar to (or perhaps identical with) that of the waking state and a phasic activity which is specific to it. However intense the cortical, reticular or pyramidal neuronal activity during P.S., it is prevented from expressing itself (except for the rapid eye movements) in appropriate tonic motor phenomena by the inhibitory reticular formation set in action by the pons. The findings we have discussed also show P.S. to be a state differing qualitatively from slow sleep. The relation between the two is nonetheless a close one, however, for slow sleop is normally a precondition for P.S. Finally, numerous observations in humans show that dreaming is the subjective equivalent of P.S. (Dement and Kleitman, 1957; M. Jouvet and D. Jouvet, 1964).

Passing from the facts to hypothetical considerations, a number of points can be made. Recollection of recent or past events during P.S. suggests that certain memory processes occur during this phase. The relatively high incidence of P.S. in the neonatal period, when the 'plastic' processes of learning are at their most active, is also indicative of a probable relationship between P.S. and the process of memory. A further sign is the presence of a particular rhythmic 6-activity in the limbic system. The relationship of such activity during wakefulness to storage of information in the CNS has been discussed by Adey (1964). Finally the parallelism between variations in P.S. and the osmolarity of the blood suggests the intervention of the neuroglia, perhaps as regulator of certain processes of protein synthesis essential to storage in the neurons; this is in keeping with the findings of Hydén and Pigon (1960).

Is it possible that the biological clock in the pons which activates P.S. may be the mechanism responsible for the complex biological processes by which we retain (or lose) the memory of past events during dreaming ? It must certainly be admitted that if molecular changes are to occur at the level of the sensory and motor neurons it would be logical for them to occur during sleep and at a moment when a safety mechanism prevents peripheral motor expression of the discharges due to protein synthesis within the neurons, for otherwise the dreamer would run the risk of behaviourally reacting to dangerous hallucinations.

Paradoxical sleep, the physiological substratum of dreaming, would thus appear as the expression of a periodic function of storing information at the molecular level. Why this mechanism expresses a need, as objectified by deprivation experiments, remains to be explained. Are we to imagine a threshold in the functional information storage processes occurring in the waking state which initiates slow sleep? A large quantity of information (external stimuli, prolonged low-frequency stimuli) does in fact quickly produce slow sleep in the intact animal (supraliminal inhibition of the Pavlovian type, hypnogenic stimulation). It is possible that, in the pons, where the majority of neurons of the central nervous system converge, certain cells are subject to a biochemical mechanism which represents a certain threshold of functional storage of information. Once this threshold has been passed, a pontine mechanism would initiate the storage process at the molecular level. It is not unreasonable to presume that such processes are cyclic and autoregulatory and that they cannot exceed a certain limit of activity of 60%, as is the case during recuperation following deprivation of paradoxical sleep.

Next page

BIBLIOGRAPHY
  1. Adey, W. R.
    (1964) ; L'élaboration et le stockage de l'information dans le système nerveux
    Actualités Neurophysiologiques, V série. M. Monnier, Editor. Masson, Paris.
  2. Bard, P., And Macht, M. B.
    (1958) ; The behaviour of chronically decerebrate cats
    Neurological Basis of Behaviour. Churchill London (p. 55-71).
  3. Berlucchi, G., Moruzzi, G., Salvi, G., And Strata, P.
    (1964) ; Pupil behaviour and ocular movements during synchronized and desynchronized sleep
    Arch. Ital. Biol., 102, 230-245.
  4. Bessman, S. P., And Skolnik, S. J.
    (1964) ; Gamma hydroxybutyrate and gamma butyrolactone. Concentration in rat tissues during anesthesia
    Science, 143, 1045-1047.
  5. Birzis, L., And Tachibana, S.
    (1964) ; Local cerebral impedance and blood flow during sleep and arousal
    Exp. Neurol., 9, 269 285.
  6. Brodal, A.
    (1957) ; The Reticular Formation of the Brain Stem. Anatomical Aspects and Functional Correlations
    Edinburgh, Oliver and Boyd.
  7. Brooks, D. C., And Bizzi, E.
    (1963) ; Brain stem electrical activity during deep sleep
    Arch. Ital. Biol., 101, 648 665.
  8. Cadilhac, J., And Passouant-Fontaine, T.
    (1962) ; Décharges épileptiques et activité électrique de veille et de sommeil dans l'hippocampe au cours de l'ontogenèse
    Physiologie de l'Hippocampe. P. Passouant, Editor. C.N.R.S. (p. 429 442).
  9. Cadilhac, J., Passouant-Fontaine, T., And Passouant, P.
    (1961) ; Modifications de l'activité de l'hippocampe suivant les divers stades du sommeil spontané chez le chat
    Rev. Neurol., 105, 171 176.
  10. Candia, O., Favale, E., Giussani, A., And Rossi, G. F.
    (1962) ; Blood pressure during natural sleep and during sleep induced by electrical stimulation of the brain stem reticular formation
    Arch. Ital. Biol., 100, 216-233.
  11. Delange, M., Castan, P., Cadilhac, J., And Passouant, P.
    (1961) ; Etude E.E.G. des divers stades du sommeil de nuit chez l'enfant. Considérations sur le stade IV ou d'activité onirique
    Rev. Neurol., 105, 176-181.
  12. Dement, W. C.
    (1958) ; The occurrence of low voltage, fast, electroencephalogram patterns during behavioral sleep in the cat
    Electroenceph. clin. Neurophysiol., 10, 291-296.
  13. Dement, W. C.
    (1960) ; The effect of dream deprivation
    Science, 13t, 1705-1707.
  14. Dement, W. C.
    (1964) ; Eye movements during sleep
    The Oculomotor System. M. B. Bender, Editor. Harper and Row, New York (p. 366-416).
  15. Dement, W. C.
    (1964) ; Does rapid eye movement sleep have a function ?
    lnternat. Symp. on the Anatomo Functional Aspects of Sleep. Lyons, 9-11 Sept. 1963; Edition C.N.R.S., in the press.
  16. Dement W. C., And Kleltman, N.
    (1957) ; The relation of eye movements during sleep to dream activity: an objective method for the study of dreaming
    J. exp. Psychol., 53, 339 346.
  17. De Robertis, E., And Gerschenfeld, H. M.
    (1961) ; Submicroscopic morphology and function of glial cells
    Int. Rev. Neurobiol., 3, 1.
  18. Edstrom, R.
    Recent developments of the blood brain barrier concept
    Int. Rev. Neurobiol. (in the press).
  19. Evarts, E. V.
    (1962) ; Activity of neurons in visual cortex of cat during sleep with low voltage fast activity
    J. Neurophysiol., 25, 812 816.
  20. Faure, J.
    (1962) ; La phase paradoxale du sommeil chez le lapin. (Ses relations neurohormonales)
    Rev. Neurol., 106, 190 197.
  21. Gerschenfeld, H. M., Wald, F., Zadunaisky, J. A., And De Robertis, E.
    (1959) ; Function of astroglia in the water-ion metabolism of the central nervous system
    Neurology, 9, 412 425.
  22. Giaquinto, S., Pompeiano, O., And Somogyi, I.
    (1964) ; Descending inhibitory influences on spinal reflexes during natural sleep
    Arch. Ital. Biol., 102, 282-308.
  23. Giarman, N. J., And Roth, R. H.
    (1964) ; Differential estimation of gamma-butyrolactone and gamma-hydroxybutyric acid in rat blood and brain
    Science, 145, 583 584.
  24. Giarman, N. J., And Schmidt, K. F.
    (1963) ; Some neurochemical aspects of the depressant action of gamma-butyrolactone on the central nervous system
    Brit. J. Pharmacol., 20, 563 568.
  25. Hermann, H., Jouvet, M., And Klein, M.
    (1964) ; Etude polygraphique du sommeil chez la tortue
    C.R. Acad. Sci. (Paris), 258, 2175 2178.
  26. Hernandez-Peon, R.
    (1963) ; Sleep induced by localized electrical or chemical stimulation of the forebrain
    Electroenceph. clin. Neurophysiol., 24, Suppl., 188-198.
  27. Hobson, J. A.
    The effect of chronic brain stem lesions on cortical and muscular activity in the cat
    Electroenceph. clin. Neurophysiol. in the press.
  28. Hobson, J. A.
    (1964) ; L'activité électrique phasique du cortex et du thalamus au cours du sommeil désynchronisé chez le chat
    C. R. Soc. Biol. (Paris), in the press.
  29. Hubel, D. H.
    (1960) ; Electrocorticograms in cats during natural sleep
    Arch. Ital. Biol., 98. 171-181.
  30. Huttenlocher, P. R.
    (1961) ; Evoked and spontaneous activity in single units of medial brain stem during natural sleep and waking
    J. Neurophysiol., 24, 451-468.
  31. Hyden, H., And Pigon, A.
    (1960) ; A cytophysiological study of the functional re]ationship between Oligodendrogial Cells and nerve cells of Deiters' nucleus
    J. Neurochem., 6, 57-72.
  32. Jeannerod, M., And Mouret, J.
    (1963) ; Etude comparative des mouvements oculaires observés chez ]e chat au cours de la veille et du sommeil
    J. Physiol. (Paris), 55, 268.
  33. Jeannerod, M., Mouret, J., And Jouvet, M.
    (1965) ; Etude de la motricité oculaire au cours de la phase paradoxale du sommeil chez le chat
    Electroenceph. clin. Neurophysiol., in the press.
  34. Jouvet, D.
    (1962) ; La phase rbombencéphalique du sommeil. Ses rapports avec l'activité onirique
    Thesis, Lyons.
  35. Jouvet, D., Valatx, J. L., And Jouvet, M.
    (1961) ; Etude polygraphique du sommeil du chaton
    C.R. Soc. Biol. (Paris), 155, 1660 1664.
  36. Jouvet, D., Vimont, P., Delorme, J. F., And Jouvet, M.
    (1964) ; Etude de la privation de phase paradoxa]e du sommei] chez le chat
    C.R. Soc. Biol. (Paris), 158, 756-759.
  37. Jouvet, M.
    (1961) ; Telencepha]ic and rhombencephalic sleep in the cat
    The Nature of Sleep, G. E. W. Wolstenholme and M. O'Connor, Editors, London (p. 188-208).
  38. Jouvet, M.
    (1962a) ; Recherches sur ]es structures nerveuses et les mécanismes responsab]es des différentes phases du sommeil physiologique
    Arch. Ital. Biol., 100, 125-206.
    TEXTE-INTEGRAL
  39. Jouvet, M.
    (1962b) ; Un appareil enregistreur automatique des phases rhombencépha]iques du sommeil chez l'animal: l'onirographe
    Rev. Neurol., 107, 269 271.
  40. Jouvet, M., Cier, A., Mounier, D., And Valatx, J. L.
    (1961) ; Effets du 4 butyrolactone et du 4 hydroxybutyrate de sodium sur l'E.E.G. et ]e comportement du chat
    C. R. Soc. Biol. (Paris) , 155, 1313 1316.
  41. Jouvet, M., And Jouvet, D.
    (1964) ; Le sommeil et les rêves chez les animaux.
    Psychiatrie animale. H. Ey, Editor. Desclée de Brovwe.
  42. Jouvet, M., Jouvet, D., And Valatx, J. L.
    (1963) ; Etude du sommeil chez le chat pontique chronique: sa suppression automatique
    C. R. Soc. Biol. (Paris), 157, 845-849.
  43. Jouvet, M., Michel, F., And Courjon, J.
    (1959) ; Sur un stade d'activité é]ectrique cérébrale rapide au cours du sommeil physio]ogique
    C.R. Soc. Biol. (Paris), 153, 1024-1028.
  44. Kanzow, E., Krause, D., And Kuehnel, H.
    (1962) ; The vasomotor system of the cerebral cortex in the phases of desynchronized E.E.G. activity during natural sleep in cats
    Pflugers Arch. ges. Physiol., 274, 593 607.
  45. Kawakami, M., And Sawyer, C. H.
    (1962) ; Effects of hormones on 'paradoxical' sleep in the rabbit
    Fed. Proc., 21, 354-359.
  46. Klein, M.
    (1963) ; Etude polygraphique et phylogénétique des différents États de Sommeil
    Thesis. Lyon, Bosc Edit.
  47. Klein, M., Michel, F., And Jouvet, M.
    (1964) ; Etude polygraphique du sommeil chez les oiseaux
    C.R. Soc. Biol. (Paris), 158, 99-103.
  48. Koch, E
    (1932) ; Die Irradiation der pressoreceptorischen Kreislaufreflexe. Klin. Wschr., 2, 225227.
  49. Lissak, K., Karmos, G., And Grastyan, E.
    (1962) ; The importance of muscular afferentation in the organization of the 'paradoxical phase' of sleep
    Abstract XXII Int. Physiol. Congr., Excerpta Medica, No. 932. Leiden.
  50. Magnes, J., Moruzzi, G., And Pompeiano, O.
    (1961) ; Synchronization of the E.E.G. produced by low-frequency electrical stimulation of the region of the solitary tract
    Arch. Ital. Biol., 99, 33-67.
  51. Magoun, H. W.
    (1950) ; Caudal and cephalic influences of the brain stem reticular formation
    Physiol. Rev., 30, 459 474.
  52. Magoun, H. W., And Rhines, R.
    (1946) ; An inhibitory mechanism in the bulbar reticular formation
    J. Neurophysiol., 9, 165-171.
  53. Michel, F., Jeannerod, M., Mouret, J., Rechtschaffen, A., And Jouvet, M.
    (1964a) ; Sur les mécanismes de l'activité de pointes au niveau du système visuel au cours de la phase paradoxale du sommeil
    C.R. Soc. Biol. (Paris), 158, 103-106.
  54. Michel, F., Rechtschaffen, A., And Vimont, P.
    (1964b) ; Activité électrique des muscles oculaires extrinsèques au cours du cycle veille sommeil
    C. R. Soc. Biol. (Paris), 158, 1 06-109.
  55. Mikiten, T., Niebyl, P., And Hendley, C.
    (1961) ; E.E.G. desynchronization during behavioural sleep associated with spike discharge from the thalamus of the cat
    Fed. Proc., 20, 327.
  56. Moruzzi, G.
    (1960) ; Synchronizing influences of the brain stem and the inhibitory mechanisms underlying the production of sleep by sensory stimulation
    Electroenceph. clin. Neurophysiol., 13 Suppl., 231-253.
  57. Moruzzi, G.
    (1964) ; Reticular influences On the E.E.G.
    Electroenceph. clin. Neurophysiol., 16, 2-17.
  58. Moruzzi, G., And Magoun, H. W.
    (1949) ; Brain stem reticular formation and activation of the E.E.G.
    Electroenceph. clin. Neurophysiol., 1, 455 473.
  59. Mouret, J., Jeannerod, M., And Jouvet, M.
    (1963) ; L'activité électrique du système visuel au cours de la phase paradoxale du sommeil chez le chat
    J. Physiol. (Paris), 55, 305-306.
  60. Naquet, R., Denavit, M., Lanoir, J., And Albe-Fessard D.
    Altérations transitoires ou définitives de zones diencéphaliques chez le chat. Leurs effets sur l'activité électrique corticale et le sommeil
    Internat. Symp. on the Anatomo-Functional Aspects of Sleep. Lyons 9 11 Sept. 1963. Edition C.N.R.S., in the press.
  61. Nauta, W. J. H.
    (1946) ; Hypothalamic regulation of sleep in rats. An experimental study
    J. Neurophysiol., 9, 285-316.
  62. Pompeiano, O.
    Ascending and descending influences of somatic afferent volleys in unrestrained cats: supraspinal inhibitory control of spinal reflexes during natural and reflexly induced sleep
    Internat. Symp. on the Anatomo-Functional Aspects of Sleep. Lyons 9-11 Sept. 1963. Edition C.N.R.S., in the press.
  63. Pompeiano, O., And Swett, J. E.
    (1962a) ; E.E.G. and behavioral manifestations of sleep induced by cutaneous nerve stimulation in normal cats
    Arch. Ital. Biol., 100, 311-342.
  64. Pompeiano, O., And Swett, J. E.
    (1962b) ; Identification of cutaneous and muscular afferent fibers producing E.E.G. synchronization or arousal in normal cats
    Arch. Ital. Biol., 100, 343-380.
  65. Pompeiano, O., And Swett, J. E.
    (1963) ; Actions of graded cutaneous and muscular afferent volleys on brain stem units in the decerebrate cerebellectomized cat
    Arch. Ital. Biol., 101, 552-583.
  66. Rechtschaffen, A., Wolpert, E. A., Dement, W. C., Mitchell, S. A., And Fisher, C.
    (1963) ; Nocturnal Sleep of Narcoleptics
    Electroenceph. clin. Neurophysiol., 15, 599 609.
  67. Reinoso-Suarez, F.
    (1961) ; Topographischer Hirnatlas der Katze.
    Darmstadt, Merck.
  68. Roffwarg, H. P., Dement, W. C., And Fisher, C.
    (1963) ; A sleep E.E.G. rapid eye movement cycle in new born infants associated with specific physiological variations
    Report of Curr. Res. Assoc. for the Psychophysiol. Study of Sleep.
  69. Rossi, G. F., Favale, E., Hara, T., Gtussani, A., And Sacco, G.
    (1961) ; Researches on the nervous mechanisms underlying deep sleep in the cat
    Arch. Ital. Biol., 99, 270 292.
  70. Rossi, G. F., Minobe, K., And Candia, O.
    (1963) ; An experimental study of the hypnogenic mechanisms of the brain stem
    Arch. Ital. Biol., 101, 470 492.
  71. Scheibel, M. E., And Scheibel, A. B.
    (1957) ; Structural substrates for integrative patterns in the brain stem reticular core. Reticular Formation of the Brain
    Henry Ford HospItal. Symposium. Boston, Little Brown and Co. (p. 31-55).
  72. Snyder, R. S., And Niemer, W. T.
    (1961) ; A Stereotaxic Atlas of the Cat Brain
    Univ. of Chic. Press.
  73. Stern, E. W., And Coxon, R. V.
    (1964) ; Osmolarity of brain tissues and its relation to brain bulk
    Anrer. J. Physiol., 206, 1-7.
  74. Tschirgt, R. D.
    (1958) ; The blood brain barrier. Biology of Neuroglia
    W. F. Windle, Editor. Springfield, C. Thomas (p. 130-138).
  75. Valatx, J. L.
    (1963) ; Ontogenèse des différents États de Sommeil. Etude comportementale E.E.G. et polygraphique chez le Chaton
    Thesis. Lyons, Annequin Edit.
  76. Valatx, J. L., Jouvet, D., And Jouvet, M.
    (1964) ; Evolution électroencéphalographique des différents états de sommeil chez le chaton
    Electroenceph. clin. Neurophysiol., 17, 218-233.
  77. Von Economo, C.
    (1929) ; Schlaftheorie
    Ergebn. Physiol., 28, 312-339.
  78. Wurtz, R. H.
    Steady potential shifts during arousal and deep sleep in the cat
    Submitted to J. Neurophysiol.