Biogenic Amines and the States of Sleep
Michel Jouvet
Science 163 (862) pages : 32-41 (1969)
TABLE OF CONTENTS

Introduction

The Four Major Concepts

Biogenic Amines and the Sleep States

Insomnia Following Selective Decrease of Cerebral Serotonin

The Problem of Paradoxical Sleep

Summary

FIGURES

PRINT
Printable version

Insomnia Following Selective Decrease of Cerebral Serotonin

Pharmacologically induced decrease. It has been shown that p-chlorophenylalanine selectively decreases the concentration of serotonin in the brain without altering the concentration of either noradrenalin or dopamine. This selective action is probably effected by inhibition of the enzyme tryptophan hydroxylase (41). In cats (42, 43), rats (44), and monkeys (45), the action of this drug has been recently studied by means of electrodes permanently implanted in the brain. After a single injection of p-chlorophenylalanine (400 milligrams per kilogram) in the cat (42) no apparent variation of behavior or of the electroencephalographic record is observed during the first 24 hours. This fact demonstrates that the drug in itself has no direct pharmacological action upon the brain. Following this period there is an abrupt decrease in the in cidence of both sleep states, and after 30 hours there is almost total insomnia, as shown by continuous quiet waking behavior, mild mydriasis, and a permanent rapid low-voltage cortical activity. The reoccurrence of sleep begins after the 60th hour and is accompanied by the appearance of continuous phasic waves in the lateral geniculate body and occipItal. cortex (similar to the PGO wave activity usually observed during the slow-wave sleep that immediately precedes paradoxical sleep and during paradoxical sleep). Discrete episodes of paradoxical sleep occur, following short periods of slow-wave sleep and even immediately following waking. Slow-wave-sleep episodes of longer duration gradually reappear at shorter intervals. Normal patterns of sleep reoccur after about 200 hours. A significant correlation has been found to exist between the decrease in slow-wave sleep and the decrease in cerebral concentrations of serotonin (at the level of serotonin-containing terminals in the rat). There was no significant alteration in the cerebral concentrations of noradrenalin or dopa mine (44) (Fig. 2).

Since p-chlorophenylalanine inhibits only the first step in the synthesis of serotonin, it is possible to bypass its blocking action and thereby reestablish the concentration of serotonin by injecting the direct precursor of serotonin. 'This has been accomplished by the injection of 5-hydroxytryptophan (which readily crosses the blood-brain barrier) following the injection of p-chlorophenylalanine. In this way it has been shown that the sleep state of the animal can be readily manipulated (42). A single injection (intravenous or peritoneal) of a very small dose of 5-hydroxytryptophan (2 to 5 milligrams per kilogram) administered when the decrease in sleep has reached its maximum (30 hours following injection of p chlorophenylalanine) results in restoration of a normal pattern of both states of sleep (Fig. 3). This normal pattern may last for a period of 6 to 10 hours, after which there is a rapid return to insomnia. In another series of experiments a control cat receiving daily doses of p-chlorophenylalanine experiences a severe and long-lasting insomnia as sociated with behavioral disturbances (anorexia, inability to walk), whereas an animal receiving balanced doses of 5-hydroxytryptophan and p-chlorophenylalanine sleeps a normal amount, or even more than normal, for several days, with no behavioral disturbances. These experiments show rather conclusively that one can manipulate sleep mechanisms at will merely by interfering with the synthesis of serotonin.

The results of this study (which are in agreement with the previous findings that serotonin injected after the injection of reserpine restores slow-wave sleep) led to the hypothesis that slow-wave sleep requires the presence of serotonin at the terminals of serotonin-containing neurons. Since other pharmacological data demonstrate that paradoxical sleep is eliminated for a long period by in jection of monoamine oxidase inhibitors (36), it is suggested that a still unknown deaminated metabolite of serotonin may be responsible for the triggering of para doxical sleep (30). The results of this pharmacological study must be critically evaluated and cannot be accepted as conclusive evidence for the existence of a serotonergic mechanism of slow-wave sleep. It must be recognized that not only the metabolism of serotonin in the brain but also that in the total periphery is altered by p-chlorophenylalanine. It is therefore difflcult to rule out a possible extracerebral factor in the dramatic insomnia which follows injection of this drug.

For this reason it is necessary to make a neurophysiological study of the role of serotonin as well as a pharmacological study. Because of the topographical localization of the serotonin-containing neurons, it was found possible to selectively destroy most of these neurons and to thus diminish the level of serotonin in the brain.

Decrease by neurophysiological techniques. By means of stereotaxic methods, the serotonin-containing neurons of the raphe system were destroyed study must be critically evaluated and in cats having electrodes permanently implanted in the brain (Fig. 4). The animals' brain waves were continuously recorded for 10 to 13 days after the operation, this being the critical period for the voiding of the serotonin-containing terminals (37). At the end of this period the cats were killed (at a fixed hour so as to avoid differences due to circadian alteration of serotonin concentration) for histological evaluation of the lesioned area of the brain and for biochemical analysis of the intact regions of the brain (telencephalon, diencephalon, mesencephalon, and spinal cord). By this method, the following information was obtained: a valid quantitative measure of the sleep states (obtained from the mean percentages of slow-wave sleep and paradoxical sleep during the 10 to 13 days of recording); a measure of the extent of the lesion (the percentage of the total raphe system destroyed), obtained by topographical analysis; and an estimate of the percentages of serotonin and noradrenalin in the brains of cats with brain lesions relative to the percentages in the brains of cats which had undergone sham operations (46).

After destruction of a large part (80 to 90 percent) of the raphe system, a continuous state of insomnia, as evidenced by the cat's behavior and by the encephalographic record, is observed during the first 3 to 4 days. In the period that follows, some slow-wave sleep is observed, but the total duration of the periods of slow-wave sleep never exceeds 10 percent of the recording time. Paradoxical sleep is never observed in these cats. Partial lesions of the raphe system (in the anterior or posterior regions) result in an insomnia which is less pronounced (due to a gradual recuperation after the first 2 days). In cats with such lesions paradoxical sleep is found to occur in association with slow wave sleep when the daily percentage of slow-wave sleep exceeds 15 percent; when the percentage is less than this, no paradoxical sleep occurs (Fig. 5). Lesioning of the rostral nuclei often results in "narcolepsy," with paradoxical sleep following directly upon the waking state. Destructions involving less than 15 percent of the raphe system provoke no significant change in the sleep states. A significant correlation between the volume of the raphe system destroyed and the percentage of sleep was thus demonstrated (47) (Fig. 6).

Biochemical analysis of cat brains in which there had been significant de struction of the raphe system revealed a significant decrease in cerebral serotonin with no variation in the concentration of noradrenalin. Thus, a significant correlation between the volume of the raphe system destroyed, the intensity of the resulting insomnia, and the decrease in cerebral serotonin was demonstrated (Fig. 6) (Table 1). In view of the neuropharmacological findings which showed that inhibition of the synthesis of serotonin (by p-chlorophenylalanine) produces total insomnia and that administration of 5-hydroxytryptophan, the precursor of serotonin, reverses this insomnia, the evidenee favoring the involvement of serotonin in sleep is exeedingly strong.

However, the intrieate and intimate bioehemieal mechanisms underlying the role of serotonin in the sleep process are as yet not well defined. Analysis has failed to reveal a significant change in the concentration of endogenous sero tonin during total deprivation of sleep (48). Nevertheless it has recently been reported that a small but significant fall in serotonin concentration accompanied by a rise in 5-hydroxyindoleacctic acid concentration occurs in the rat hrain during deprivation of paradoxical sleep (49). It has also been demonstrated that the brain concentration of serotonin is higher during the day (when the rat sleeps most of the time) than during the night (22). Only through in vivo study of the tunrnover of serotonin can understanding be achieved of the intricate processes underlying the serotonergic mechanism of sleep.

Next page

REFERENCES AND NOTES
  • 1- G Moruzzi and H. W. Magoun
    Electroencephalog. Clin. Neurophysiol. 1, 445 (1949).
  • 2 - F. Bremer
    in Brain Mechanisms and Consciousness, E. D. Adrian, F. Bremer, H. H. Jasper, Eds. (Blackwell, Oxford, 1954), pp. 137-162; F. Bremer, Schweiz. Arch. Neurol. Neurochir. Psychiat. 86, 34 (1960); N. Kleitman, Sleep and Wakefulness (Univ. of Chicago Press, Chicago, 1963); D. B. Lindsley, in Handbook of Physiology and Neurophysiology, J. Field H. W. Magoun, V. E. Hall, Eds.(American Physiological Society, Washington, 20 D.C., 1960), vol. 1, p. 1553; for the historical development of the passive hypothesis of sleep, see G. Moruzzi, Proc. Amer. Phil. Soc. 108, 19 (1964).
  • 3- See P. L. Parmeggiani
    in Progress in Brain Research, Topics in Basic Neurology, W. Batgmann and J. L. Schadé, Eds. (Elsevier, Amsterdam, 1964), references pp. 180-190.
  • 4 - The mean percentages of slow-wave sleep and paradoxical sleep in the adult cat are, respectively, 53 and 16 percent of a 24-hour day; the mean duration of paradoxical-sleep episodes is 6 minutes. See F. Delorme, P. Vimont, D. Jouvet, Compt. Rend. Soc. Biol. 158, 2128 (1964); M. B. Sterman, T. Knauss, D.Lehmann, C. D. Clemente Electroencephalog. Clin. Neurophysiol. 19, 509 (1965).
  • 5 - C. Batini, G. Moruzzi, M. Palestini, G. F Rossi, A. Zanchetti
    Science 128, 30 (1958); C Batini F Magni, M. Palestini G F. Rossi A. Zanchetti, Arch. Ital. Biol. 97,13 (1959).
  • 6 - M. Bonvallet and V. Bloch
    Science 133, 1133 (1961);G. F Rossi, K. Minobe, O Candia Arch. Ital. Biol. 101, 470 (1963)- A. Camacho Evangelista and F. Reinoso-Suarez, Science 146, 268 (1964); G. Moruzzi, Harvey Lectures Ser. 58, 233 (1963); , Electroencephalog. Clin. Neurophysiol. 16, 2 (1964).
  • 7 - F. Fontana
    Dei moti pell'iride (Giusti, Lucca, 1765); R. Klaue, J. Psychol. Neurol. Leipzig 47, 510 (1937).
  • 8 - E. Aserinsky and N. Kleitman
    J. Appl. Physiol. 8, 1 (1955); W, C. Dement and N. Kleitman, Electroencephalog. Clin. Neurophysiol. 9, 673 (1957); W. C. Dement, ibid. 10, 291 (1958), M. Jouvet, F. Michel, J. Courjon, Compt. Rend. Soc. Biol. 153, 101 (1959).
  • 9 - F. Snyder
    Amer. J. Psychiat. 122, 377 (1965); W. C. Dement, in New Directions in Psychology, T, M. Newcomb, Ed (Holt, Rinehart, and Winston, New York, 1965), vol. 2, p. 135; M. Jouvet Physiol. Rev. 47, 117 (1967); W. P. Koella Sleep (Thomas, Springffeld, ID.,1967 ); E. Hartmann, The Biology of Dreaming (Thomas, Springfield, 111., 1967).
  • 10 - Slow-wave sleep is also called light sleep,deep sleep, synchronized sleep, and non-rapid eye-movement sleep.
  • 11 - Paradoxical sleep is also called activated sleep, deep sleep, desynchronized sleep, rapid eye-moyement or REM sleep, para sleep, and rhombencephalic sleep. Since dreaming occurs almost exclusively during this sleep state, it is also called dreaming sleep or D-state.
  • 12 - M. Jeannerod J. Mouret M. Jouvet
    J. Physiol. Paris 57, 255 (1965); F. Michel, M. Jeannerod, J. Mouret, A. Rechstchaffen, M. Jouvet Compt. Rend. Soc. Biol. 158, 103 (1964;- J. Mouret, M. Jeannerod, M. Jouvet, J. Physiol. Paris 55, 305 (1963); M. Jeannerod, thesis. University of Lyons (1965).
  • 13 - D. C. Brooks and E. Bizzi
    Arch. Ital. Biol. 101, 648 (1963); D. C. Brooks, paper presented before the Association for the Psychophysiological Study of Sleep, Denver, 1968.
  • 14 - D. Peyrethon-Duzan, J. Peyrethon, M. Jouvet
    Comp. Rend. Soc. Biol. 161, 2530 (1967).
  • 15 - E. Bizzi and D, C. Brooks
    Arch. Ital. Biol. 101, 666 (1963), O. Benoit J. Physiol. Paris 56, 259 (1964); O, Pompeiano and A. R. Morrison, Arch. Ital. Biol. 103, 569 (1965); M. Jouvet, M. Jeannerod, J. F. Delorme, Compt. Rend. Soc. Biol. 159, 1599 (1965).
  • 16 - J. L. Valatx, D. Jouvet, M. Jouvet
    Electroencephalog. Clin. Neurophysiol. 17, 218 (1964); J. Cadilhac, T. Passouant-Fontaine, P. Passouant, J. Physiol. Paris 54, 305 (1962); D. Jouvet-Mounier, thesis University of Lyons 1968); and L. Astic, Compt. Rend. Soc. Biol. 156, 1411 (1967); A. Shimizu and H. E. Him wich, Electroencephalog. Clin. Neurophysiol. 24, 307 (1968) H. P. Roffwarg, J. N. Muzio, W. C. Dement Science 152, 604 (1966).
  • 17 - J. Peyrethon and D. Peyrethon-Duzan
    Compt. Rend. Soc. Biol. 161, 2533 (1967).
  • 18 - H. Hermann, M. Jouvet, M. Klein
    Compt. Rend. 258, 2175 (1964); E. S. Tauber, H. P Roffwarg, E. D. Weitzman, Nature 212, 1612 (1966)- A. Rechtschaffen, M. Bassan, S. Ledecky-Tanecek, paper presented before the Association for the Psychophysiological Study of Sleep, Denver, 1968.
  • 19 - M. Klein F. Michel, M. Jouvet
    Compt. Rend. Soc. Biol. 158, 99 (1964); T. Ookawa and J. Gotoh, Poultry Sci., 43, 1603 (1964); V. Tradardi, Arch. Ital. Biol. 104, 516 (1966; M. A. Corner, J. J, Peters, P. Rutgers van der Loeff, Brain Res, 2, 274 (1966).
  • 20 - F Snyder
    Amer. J. Psychiat. 123 121 (1966).
  • 21 - E. Hartmann, J. Bernstein, C. Wilson
    Psychophysiology 4, 389 (1968).
  • 22 - P. Albrecht, M. B. Vissher, J. J. Bittner, F. Halberg
    Proc. Soc. Exp. Biol. 92, 703 (1956); L. E. Scheving, W. H. Harrison, P. Gordon,J. E. Pauly, Amer. J. Physiol. 214, 166 (1968) .
  • 23 - F. O. Schmitt
    in Macro-molecular Specificity and Biological Memory, F. O. Schmitt,Ed. (M.l.T. Press, Cambridge, 1962).
  • 24 - W. Dement
    Science 131, 1705 (1960); D. Jouvet, P. Vimont, F. Delorme, M. Jouvet, Compt. Rend. Soc. Biol. 158, 756 (1964); P. Vimont, D. Jouvet, J. F. Delorme, Electroencephalog. Clin. Neurophysiol. 20, 439 (1966); W. C. Dement, P, Henry, H. Cohen, J. Ferguson, Sleep and Altered States of Consciousness, S. S. Kety E. V. Evarts, H. L. Williams, Eds. (Williams and Wilkins, Baltimore,1967) .
  • 25 - C. E. Spooner and W. D. Winters
    Experientia 21, 256 (1965); In. J. Neuropharmacol. 5, 217 (1966); ibid. 6, 109 (1967).
  • 26 - W. P. Koella C. M. Trunca, J. S. Szicman
    Life Sci. 4, 1;3 (1965); W. P. Koella and J.S. Czicman, Amer. J. Physiol. 211, 926 (1966); I X. Lebedur and R. Tissot, Electroencephalog. Clin. Neurophysiol. 20 (1965).
  • 27 - W. Feldberg and S. L. Sherwood
    J. Physiol. London 123, 148 (1954)- P. B. Bradley and A. J. Hance, ibid. 132, 50 (1956); E. R. John, B. M. Wenzel, R. D. Tschirgi, J. Pharmacol. Exp.Therap. 123, 193 (1958).
  • 28 - D. F. Bogdanski, H. Weissbach, S. Udenfriend
    J. Pharmacol. Exp. Therap. 122, 182 (1958); M. Monnier and R. Tissot, Helv. Physiol. Pharmacol. Acta 16 255 (1958); E. Costa, G. R. Pscheidt, W. G. van Meter, H.E. Himwich, J. Pharmacol. Exp. Therap. 130,81 (1960).
  • 29 - F. Delorme
    thesis, University of Lyons (1966).
  • 30 - M. Jouvet
    in Sleep and Alrered States of Consciousness, S, S. Kety, E. V. Evarts, H. L. Williams, Eds. (Williams and Wilkins, Baltimore, 1967),
  • 31 - B. J. Key and E. Marley
    Electroencephalog. Clin. Neurophysiol. 14, 90 (1962); C. E. Spooner and W. D. Winters, Int. J. Neuropharmacol. 6, 109 (1967); M. Momnier and R. Tissot, Helv. Physiol. Pharmacol. Acta 16,255 (1958).
  • 32 - H. Blaschko, K. H. Burn, H. Langemann
    Brit. J. Pharmacol. 5, 431 (1950).
  • 33 - V. Havlicek
    Int. J. Neuropharmacol. 6, 83 (1967) .
  • 34 - The state of "tranquility" produced by reserpine led to the hypothesis that serotonin could be involved in parasympathetic (trophotropic) mechanisms of sleep; see B. A. Brodie. P. A. Shore, A. Pletscher, Science 123, 993 (1956), B. A. Brodie, K. F. Finger, F.B. Orlans, G. P. Quinn, F. Sulser, J. Pharmacol. Exp. Therap. 129, 250 (1960).
  • 35 - J. Matsumoto and M. Jouvet
    Compt. Rend. Soc. Biol. 158, 2135 (1964) F. Delorme, M. Jeannerod, M. Jouvet, ibid. 159, 900 (1965).
  • 36 - M. Jouvet, P. Vimont, J. F. Delorme
    ibid p. 1595.
  • 37 - B. Falck
    in Progress in Brain Research, H.G. Himwich, Ed. (Elsevier, Amsterdam, 1964), p. 28- B. Falck N. A. Hillarp, G. Thieme, A Torp, J. Histochem. Cytochem. 10, 348 (1962); A. Dahlstrom and K. Fuxe, Acta Physiol. Scand. Suppl. 232, 1 (1964); ibid. 247, 5 (1965); K. Fuxe, Z. Zelforsch.Mikroskop. Anat. Abt. Histochemie 65, 573 ( 1965 ) .
  • 38 - A. Brodal E. Taber, F. Walberg
    J. Comp. Neurol., 239 (1960).
  • 39 - A. Heller and R. Y. Moore
    J. Pharmacol. Exp. Therap. 150 1 (1965), J. A. Harvey, A. Heller, R. Y. Moore, ibid. 140, 103 (1963); A. Heller, J. A. Haney, R. Y. Moore, Biochem. Pharmacol. 11, 859 (1962).
  • 40 - It is not surprising that, if this procedure is not followed, attempts to correlate serotonin concentrations in rostral parts of the brain with the electrical activity of the cortex after eitber midpontine transection (which leads to fast, low-voltage activity) or rostropontine transection (which leads to synchronized acticity) have produced negative results in short term experiments (R. A. Antonelll, G. Bertac cini, P. Mantegazzini, J. Neurochem. 8, 157 (1961))
  • 41 - K. B. Koe and A. Weissman
    J. Pharmacol. Exp. Therap, 154, 499 ( 1967 ) .
  • 42 - F. Delorme, L. Froment, M. Jouvet
    Compt. Rend. Soc. Biol. 160, 2347 (1966); J. Mouret, J. L. Froment, P. Bobillier, M. Jouvet, J. Physiol. Paris 59, 463 (1967). p-Chloromethamphetamin has been shown to produce a selective decrease of serotonin [A. Pletscher, G.Bartholini, H. Bruderer, W. P. Burkard, K. F. Gey, J. Pharmacol. 145, 344 (1964)]. Also, my associates and I found that this drug induces a very marked arousal which coincides with the depletion of brain serotonin. However, a secondary injection of 5-hydroxytryptophan does not restore normal sleep and has always been followed shonly by the death of the animal.
  • 43 - W. P. Koella
    paper presented before the American College of Neuropsychopharmacology, Puerto Rico, 1967.
  • 44 - J. Mouret, P, Bobillier, M. Jouvet
    Compt. Rend. Soc. Biol. 161, 1600 (1967); J. Mouret, P. Bobillier N, Frachon, A. Vilpulla, M. Jouvet European J. Pharmacol., in press; C. Torda Brain Res. 6, 375 (1967).
  • 45 - T. J Crowley, E. Smith, 0. F. Lewis
    paper presented before the Association for Psychophysiological Study of Sleep, Denver, 1968; E. D, Weitzman, M. Rapport, P. McGregor, J. Jacoby, Science 160, 1361 (1968).
  • 46 M. Jouvet and J. Renault
    Compt Rend. Soc. Biol. 160, 1461 (1966); M. Jouvet, P. Bobillier, J F. Pujol, J. Renault, ibid., p. 2343; M. Jouvet, in Advances in Pharmacology, S. Garattini and P. A. Shore, Eds. (Academic Press, New York, 1968 ); J, Renault, thesis, University of Lyons (1967).
  • 47 - These results may explain the relative insomnia following mid-pontine transections (4), since destruction of the raphe system caudal to this plane would produce the same amount of sleep (20 percent). The insomnia that follows surgical splitting of the brain stem may also be explained by these results as this procedure (sagittal transection) inevitably involves the destruction of numerous raphe neurons [F. Michel and M. P. Roffwarg, Experientia 23, 126 (1967); M. Mancia, T. Desiraju, G. S. Chhina, Electroencephalog. Clin. Neurophysiol. 24, 409 (1968)].
  • 48 - E. Hartmann and D. Freedman
    paper presented before the Association for the Psychophysiological Study of Sleep, Gainesvflle,Florida 1966- E. L. Bliss, in Sleep and Altered States of Consciousness, S. S. Kety, E.V. Evarts, H. L. Williams, Eds. (Williams and Wilkins, Baltimore, 1967).
  • 49 - E. Weiss, B. Bordwell, M, Seeger, J. Lee, W. C. Dement, J. Barchas
    paper presented before the Association for Psychophysiological Study of Sleep, Denver, 1968.
  • 50 - M. Jouvet
    Arch. Ital. Biol. 100, 125 (1962).
    FULL TEXT
  • 51 - G Carli and A. Zanchett
    i
    ibid. 103, 751 (1965)- G. Rossi Electroencephalog. Clin. Neurophysiol. 14, 428 (1962).
  • 52 - P. H. Hashimoto, T. Maeda, K, Toru, N.Shimizu
    Med. J. Osaka Univ. 12, 425 (1962).
  • 53 - J. Mouret, N. Frachon, A. Vilpulla, M. Jouvet
    Compt. Rend. Soc. Biol., in press.
  • 54 - M. Jouvet and F. Delorme
    ibid. 159, 895 (1965).
  • 55 - B. Roussel
    thesis, University of Lyons, (1967); A Buguet, P. Bobillier M. Jouvet Compt. Rend. Soc. Biol. 161, 2537 (1967).
  • 56 - It is interesting to note that, through administration for 6 to 12 weeks of synthetic diet free of phenylalanine and tyrosine (the aminoacids that are precursors of dopamine and noradrenalin) to patients with inoperable cancer, a systematic and selective decline of paradoxical sleep was obtained which was immediately reversed when tyrosine was there after added to the diet (H. L. Williams, B. K. Lester, J. D. Coulter, paper read before the Association for Psychophysiological Study of Sleep, Denver, 1968).
  • 57 - E. D. Weitzman, P. McGregor, C. Moore, J. Jacoby
    paper presented before the Association for Psychophysiological Study of Sleep, Denver, 1968; T. J. Crowley, E. S. Smith, O. F. Lewis, ibid.
  • 58 - S. Spector, A. Sjoerdsma, S. Udenfriend
    J. Pharmacol 147, 86 (1965).
  • 59 - J. L. Froment, D. Peyrethon-Duzan, M. Jouvet
    Compt. Rend. Soc. Biol., in press.
  • 60 - M. Goldstein, B. Anagnoste, E. Lauber, M.R. McKereghare
    Life Sci. Oxford 3, 763 (1964) .
  • 61 - A. Carlsson
    in Biogenic Amines, H. E. Himwich and W. A. Himwich Eds. (Elsevier Amsterdam, ]964), pp. 9-2;.
  • 62 - D. Peyrethon-Duzan, J Peyrethon, M. Jouvet
    Compt. Rend. Soc. Biol. IC2, 116 (1968).
  • 63 - J. F. Pujol, J. Mouret, M. Jouvet, J. Glowinski
    Science 159 112 (1968).
  • 64 - T. Tokizane
    in Progress in Brain Research, T. Tokizane and J. P. Schade, Eds. (Elsevier, Amsterdam, 1966).
  • 65 - N. Khazan and C. H. Sawyer
    Psychopharmacologia 5, 457 (1964).
  • 66 - R. George, W. L. Haslett, D. J. Jenden
    Int. J. Neuropharmacol. 3, 541 (1964).
  • 67 - J. H. Burn and M. J. Rand.
    J. Rev. Pharmacol. 5, 163 (1965).
  • 68 - This and our related studies are supported by a grant from the Direction des Recherches et Moyens d'Essais, l'Institut National de la Santé et de la Recherche Médicale, Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique, and a grant (E.O.A.R. 62-67) from the European Office of Aerospace Research. I thank B. E. Jones for invaluable assistance in editing the English version of this article.